Downloaded from UvA-DARE, the institutional repository of the University of Amsterdam (UvA)

Vergelijkbare documenten
Now is the Time for Reward! The Developmental Relationship Between Cognitive-Motivational Factors and Adolescent Substance Use T.

IMPLICIETE TRAININGEN: Een nieuwe manier van interventies

When things are getting out of hand: Prevalence, assessment, and treatment of substance use disorder(s) and violent behavior Kraanen, F.L.

Understanding the role of health literacy in self-management and health behaviors among older adults Geboers, Bas

(SOCIALE) ANGST, GEPEST WORDEN EN PSYCHOLOGISCHE INFLEXIBILITEIT 1

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Developmental changes in cognitive control Smulders, S.F.A. Link to publication

Today's talented youth field hockey players, the stars of tomorrow? Gemser, Marije

Prof. dr. R.C.M.E. Engels is hoogleraar ontwikkelingspsychopathologie bij de Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen.

University of Groningen. Safe and Sound van den Bosch, Kirsten Anna-Marie

Motivatiedeficit, onmacht of een dubbeltje op zijn kant?

Cognitive Bias Modification (CBM): "Computerspelletjes" tegen Angst, Depressie en Verslaving

Verschil in Perceptie over Opvoeding tussen Ouders en Adolescenten en Alcoholgebruik van Adolescenten

Melatonin treatment and light therapy for chronic sleep onset insomnia in children van Maanen, A.

Karen J. Rosier - Brattinga. Eerste begeleider: dr. Arjan Bos Tweede begeleider: dr. Ellin Simon

University of Groningen. Pieces of the Puzzle Vissia, Eline Margreta

Fidelity of a Strengths-based method for Homeless Youth

Het executief en het sociaal cognitief functioneren bij licht verstandelijk. gehandicapte jeugdigen. Samenhang met emotionele- en gedragsproblemen

Het Effect van Verschil in Sociale Invloed van Ouders en Vrienden op het Alcoholgebruik van Adolescenten.

The utility and effectiveness of an educational computer game aimed at improving ineffective learning behavior in preschool children Veenstra, Baukje

Running head: OPVOEDSTIJL, EXTERNALISEREND PROLEEMGEDRAG EN ZELFBEELD

Chapter 4 Understanding Families. In this chapter, you will learn

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Keeping youth in play Spruit, A. Link to publication

Psychometrische Eigenschappen van de Youth Anxiety Measure for DSM-5 (YAM-5) Psychometric Properties of the Youth Anxiety Measure for DSM-5 (YAM-5)

Alcoholgebruik en effecten tijdens adolescentie

Code Course name block Ects International Organizations Advanced Project management * Cross Cultural HRM 3 6

Master Thesis. Early Career Burnout Among Dutch Nurses: Comparing Theoretical Models. Using an Item Response Approach.

University of Groningen. Dilemmas in child protection Bartelink, Cora

Intercultural Mediation through the Internet Hans Verrept Intercultural mediation and policy support unit

Screening van gedragsproblemen en consequenties hiervan op effect van interventies. Walter Matthys

University of Groningen

Code Cursusnaam block Ects Organization Theory Organization Development Relations and Networks of Organizations 4 6

Tinnitus Bartels, Hilke

Nederlandse samenvatting

University of Groningen. Who cares? Kamstra, Aafke

DE MASTEROPLEIDING EDUCATION AND CHILD STUDIES (PER SPECIALISATIE) IN SCHEMA. Child and Family Science (instromen in september)

Centrum Ganspoel. Special school as resource centre for mainstreaming: developing networks & cooperation and exchange strategies

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

Downloaded from UvA-DARE, the institutional repository of the University of Amsterdam (UvA)

De Relatie tussen Angst en Psychologische Inflexibiliteit. The Relationship between Anxiety and Psychological Inflexibility.

Impliciete en expliciete cognities als voorspellers van drinkgedrag in de vroege adolescentie.

Alcoholgebruik, Alcohol-Specifieke Regels en Impliciete Opvoedcognities

Citation for published version (APA): van der Put, C. E. (2011). Risk and needs assessment for juvenile delinquents

Running head: MINDFULNESS, CONTINGENTE ZELFWAARDERING EN DEPRESSIE 1. De Invloed van een Gecombineerde Mindfulnessbehandeling op

Neurostimulation in alcohol dependence: The effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cognitive functioning and craving Jansen, Jochem

University of Groningen

CVA zorg, topsport voor ons allemaal. Dinsdag 11 april 2017

Assessing writing through objectively scored tests: a study on validity. Hiske Feenstra Cito, The Netherlands

Running head: BREAKFAST, CONSCIENTIOUSNESS AND MENTAL HEALTH 1. The Role of Breakfast Diversity and Conscientiousness in Depression and Anxiety

University of Groningen. Structured diabetes care in general practice Fokkens, Sudara Andrea

Citation for published version (APA): Holwerda, A. (2013). Work outcome in young adults with disabilities Groningen: s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Jambroes, M. (2015). The public health workforce: An assessment in the Netherlands

University of Groningen. On physical functioning after pediatric burns Disseldorp, Laurien Maria

Positieve, Negatieve en Depressieve Subklinische Psychotische Symptomen en het Effect van Stress en Sekse op deze Subklinische Psychotische Symptomen

Prof. dr. A. M. T. Bosman. Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen Sectie Orthopedagogiek van Leren en Ontwikkeling

Anxiety, fainting and gagging in dentistry: Separate or overlapping constructs? van Houtem, C.M.H.H.

Biodiversity responses to climate and land-use change: A historical perspective Aguirre Gutierrez, J.

Beïnvloedt Gentle Teaching Vaardigheden van Begeleiders en Companionship en Angst bij Verstandelijk Beperkte Cliënten?

Impliciete Cognitie en Verslaving. Theoretische Inzichten en Praktische Toepassingen

Invloed van het aantal kinderen op de seksdrive en relatievoorkeur

DE MASTEROPLEIDING EDUCATION AND CHILD STUDIES (PER SPECIALISATIE) IN SCHEMA. Child and Family Science*

PrOP Tussen Je Oren. gebaseerd op het PrOP-model. the PrOP-model. Mariëtte J.C.P. van der Stappen

Lichamelijke factoren als voorspeller voor psychisch. en lichamelijk herstel bij anorexia nervosa. Physical factors as predictors of psychological and

Het samenspel van genen en omgeving: Relevantie voor de Jeugdgezondheidszorg

The Effect of Gender, Sex Drive and Autonomy. on Sociosexuality. Invloed van Sekse, Seksdrive en Autonomie. op Sociosexualiteit

Ouderlijke Controle en Angst bij Kinderen, de Invloed van Psychologische Flexibiliteit

Mindfulness - de 8-weekse training in vogelvlucht

STIGMATISERING VAN PATIENTEN MET LONGKANKER 1. Stigmatisering van Patiënten met Longkanker: De Rol van Persoonlijke Relevantie voor de Waarnemer

The Influence of Video Interaction Guidance on the. G.F. van Haaften

Denken en Doen Doen of Denken Het verband tussen seksueel risicovol gedrag en de impulsieve en reflectieve cognitie.

University of Groningen. Stormy clouds in seventh heaven Meijer, Judith Linda

Modererende Rol van Seksuele Gedachten. Moderating Role of Sexual Thoughts. C. Iftekaralikhan-Raghubardayal

SUMMARY not show a significant difference in decrease of drinking at post-test compared with the control group. The results did not yield support for

Evaluatie van de nodige vaardigheden in diabetesmanagement.

Wat is de Modererende Rol van Consciëntieusheid, Extraversie en Neuroticisme op de Relatie tussen Depressieve Symptomen en Overeten?

Hoge mate van impulsiviteit als risicofactor voor alcoholgebruik onder adolescenten uit een risicogroep

Motivationele oriëntatie bij sociale vergelijking: Wanneer, waarom en met wie vergelijken?

The characteristics of excellent professionals: Input for honours programs. Marjolein Heijne-Penninga, Inge Wijkamp, Marca Wolfensberger

Relatie tussen Persoonlijkheid, Opleidingsniveau, Leeftijd, Geslacht en Korte- en Lange- Termijn Seksuele Strategieën

The Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory TM Version 4 (PedsQL TM 4.0)

Geslacht, Emotionele Ontrouw en Seksdrive. Gender, Emotional Infidelity and Sex Drive

De Relatie tussen Mindfulness en Psychopathologie: de Mediërende. Rol van Globale en Contingente Zelfwaardering

MINDFULNESS, ZELFASPECTEN EN WELZIJN 1. Bewust (wel)zijn? De mediërende rol van het zelf in de relatie tussen mindfulness en psychologisch welbevinden

Voorspellers van Leerbaarheid en Herstel bij Cognitieve Revalidatie van Patiënten met Niet-aangeboren Hersenletsel

Summary 124

University of Groningen. Visual semantics Schie, Hein Thomas van

Citation for published version (APA): Faber, A. (2006). Stimulant treatment in children: A Dutch perspective s.n.

University of Groningen. Injury prevention in team sport athletes Dallinga, Joan

Alcoholpreventie in Nederland en Zweden: It takes two to tango

Het Effect van Cliëntgerichte Speltherapie op Internaliserende Problematiek bij. Kinderen: Affect als Moderator

University of Groningen Educational value of digital examination

Denkstijl, Responsstijl en Comorbiditeit bij. Jongeren met Externaliserende Problematiek in. Residentiële Behandelcentra

Longitudinal Driving Behaviour At Incidents And The Influence Of Emotions. Raymond Hoogendoorn

Onderzoek naar werkzaamheid schematherapie bij borderline persoonlijkheidsstoornis en alcoholafhankelijkheid

Cognitieve Bias Modificatie van Interpretatiebias bij Faalangstige Studenten

Work. Invloed van Careeradaptability op de Relatie tussen. Approach Avoidance Temperament and Engagement

De Relatie tussen Hechting en Welbevinden bij Ouderen: De mediërende Invloed van Mindfulness en Zingeving

SLACHTOFFER CYBERPESTEN, COPING, GEZONDHEIDSKLACHTEN, DEPRESSIE. Cyberpesten: de implicaties voor gezondheid en welbevinden van slachtoffers en het

De rol van ouders in de ontwikkeling van alcoholgebruik onder adolescenten

Verslaving bij adolescenten met zwakke EF

Seksdrive, Stresscoping en Extrinsieke Ambitie : De Verschillen tussen Mannen en Vrouwen. Sexdrive, Stresscoping and Extrinsic Ambition :

Transcriptie:

Downloaded from UvA-DARE, the institutional repository of the University of Amsterdam (UvA) http://hdl.handle.net/11245/2.161587 File ID Filename Version uvapub:161587 Back matter final SOURCE (OR PART OF THE FOLLOWING SOURCE): Type PhD thesis Title Now is the time for reward! The developmental relationship between cognitive-motivational factors and adolescent substance use Author(s) T. Janssen Faculty FMG: Psychology Research Institute Year 2015 FULL BIBLIOGRAPHIC DETAILS: http://hdl.handle.net/11245/1.477411 Copyright It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content licence (like Creative Commons). UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http://dare.uva.nl) (pagedate: 2015-06-08)

REFERENCES

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishing, Inc. Ames, S. L., Sussman, S., Dent, C. W., & Stacy, A. W. (2005). Implicit cognition and dissociative experiences as predictors of adolescent substance use. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 31, 129-162. Arnett, J. J. (2005). The developmental context of substance use in emerging adulthood. Journal of Drug Issues, 35, 235-254. Arnett, P. A., & Newman, J. P. (2000). Gray's three-arousal model: An empirical investigation. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 1171-1189. Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2010). Multiple imputation with mplus. Retrieved from https://www.statmodel.com/download/imputations7.pdf Ataya, A. F., Adams, S., Mullings, E., Cooper, R. M., Attwood, A. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2012). Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 121, 148-151. Babor, T. F., Higgins-Biddle, J. C., Saunders, J. B., & Monteiro, M. G. (2001). The alcohol use disorders identification test: Guidelines for use in primary care. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556-559. Bardone, A. M., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Dickson, N., & Silva, P. A. (1996). Adult mental health and social outcomes of adolescent girls with depression and conduct disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 811-829. Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65-94. 175

References Barnes, G. M., Reifman, A. S., Farrell, M. P., & Dintcheff, B. A. (2000). The effects of parenting on the development of adolescent alcohol misuse: A Six Wave latent growth model. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62, 175-186. Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1458-1463. Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E., & Aldridge, J. W. (2009). Dissecting components of reward: liking, wanting, and learning. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 9, 65-73. Blakemore, S., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 296-312. Boelema, S. (2014). Alcohol use in adolescence: A longitudinal study of its effect on cognitive functioning (Ph.D.). Available from http://content.presspage.com/uploads/169/proefschriftsaraiboelemauniversiteitutrecht. pdf. doi:978-90-393-6238-9 Boendermaker, W. J., Prins, P. J., & Wiers, R. W. (2015). Cognitive bias modification for adolescents with substance use problems can serious games help? Manuscript in Submission. Böhning, D., Dietz, E., & Schlattmann, P. (1997). Zero-inflated count models and their applications in public health and social science. Applications of Latent Trait and Latent Class Models in the Social Sciences.Waxman Publishing Co, Bollen, K. A., & Curran, P. J. (2006). Latent curve models: A structural equation perspective. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons. Brenhouse, H. C., & Andersen, S. L. (2008). Delayed extinction and stronger reinstatement of cocaine conditioned place preference in adolescent rats, compared to adults. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122, 460-465. 176

Brown, S. A., Tapert, S. F., Granholm, E., & Delis, D. C. (2000). Neurocognitive functioning of adolescents: Effects of protracted alcohol use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 164-171. Bunge, S. A., & Wright, S. B. (2007). Neurodevelopmental changes in working memory and cognitive control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 243-250. Carskadon, M. A., & Acebo, C. (1993). A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development. Journal of Adolescent Health, 14, 190-195. Casey, B., & Jones, R. M. (2010). Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: Implications for substance use disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 1189-1201. Castellanos Ryan, N., O'Leary Barrett, M., Sully, L., & Conrod, P. (2013). Sensitivity and specificity of a brief personality screening instrument in predicting future substance use, emotional, and behavioral problems: 18 Month predictive validity of the substance use risk profile scale. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 37, E281-E290. Castellanos Ryan, N., Rubia, K., & Conrod, P. J. (2011). Response inhibition and reward response bias mediate the predictive relationships between impulsivity and sensation seeking and common and unique variance in conduct disorder and substance misuse. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35, 140-155. Castellanos, N. (2009). Substance misuse and conduct disorder in adolescence: The role of disinhibition in the onset of behavioural problems and their comorbidity. (Unpublished Doctoral dissertation). King's College, London. Chambers, R. A., Taylor, J. R., & Potenza, M. N. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1041-1052. 177

References Christiansen, P., Cole, J. C., Goudie, A. J., & Field, M. (2012). Components of behavioural impulsivity and automatic cue approach predict unique variance in hazardous drinking. Psychopharmacology, 219, 501-510. Collins, L. M., & Lanza, S. T. (2010). Latent class and latent transition analysis: With applications in the social, behavioral, and health sciences Wiley. Collins, L. M., Schafer, J. L., & Kam, C. (2001). A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in modern missing data procedures. Psychological Methods, 6, 330-351. Conklin, C. A., & Perkins, K. A. (2005). Subjective and reinforcing effects of smoking during negative mood induction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 153-164. Conrod, P. J., Castellanos, N., & Mackie, C. (2008). Personality targeted interventions delay the growth of adolescent drinking and binge drinking. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 181-190. Conrod, P. J., Castellanos-Ryan, N., & Strang, J. (2010). Brief, personality-targeted coping skills interventions and survival as a non-drug user over a 2-year period during adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 85-93. Conrod, P. J., Pihl, R. O., Stewart, S. H., & Dongier, M. (2000). Validation of a system of classifying female substance abusers on the basis of personality and motivational risk factors for substance abuse. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 14, 243-256. Cooper, M. L. (1994). Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 6, 117-128. Cooper, M. L., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Mudar, P. (1995). Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 990-1005. Coskunpinar, A., & Cyders, M. A. (2013). Impulsivity and substance-related attentional bias: A meta-analytic review. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 133, 1-14. 178

Cox, W. M., Fadardi, J. S., & Pothos, E. M. (2006). The addiction-stroop test: Theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 443-476. Cragg, L., & Nation, K. (2007). Self-ordered pointing as a test of working memory in typically developing children. Memory, 15, 526-535. Crego, A., Holguín, S. R., Parada, M., Mota, N., Corral, M., & Cadaveira, F. (2009). Binge drinking affects attentional and visual working memory processing in young university students. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33, 1870-1879. Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 636-650. Crone, E. A., & Ridderinkhof, R. K. (2011). The developing brain: From theory to neuroimaging and back. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 101-109. Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evaluations: A social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 97-104. Cyders, M. A., & Coskunpinar, A. (2011). Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: Is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 965-982. Danielsson, A., Wennberg, P., Tengström, A., & Romelsjö, A. (2010). Adolescent alcohol use trajectories: Predictors and subsequent problems. Addictive Behaviors, 35, 848-852. De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., Baeyens, F., & Hermans, D. (2001). On the generality of the affective simon effect. Cognition & Emotion, 15, 189-206. De Wit, H. (2009). Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: A review of underlying processes. Addiction Biology, 14, 22-31. DiCiccio, T. J., & Efron, B. (1996). Bootstrap confidence intervals. Statistical Science, 11, 189-212. 179

References Dovidio, J. F., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). New technologies for the direct and indirect assessment of attitudes. Questions about Questions: Inquiries into the Cognitive Bases of Surveys, 21, 204-237. Dovis, S., Van der Oord, S., Wiers, R. W., & Prins, P. J. (2012). Can motivation normalize working memory and task persistence in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? the effects of money and computer-gaming. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 669-681. Du, W., Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2002). Cross-cultural comparisons of discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Record, 52, 405-416. Ducci, F., & Goldman, D. (2008). Genetic approaches to addiction: Genes and alcohol. Addiction, 103, 1414-1428. Duka, T., Gentry, J., Malcolm, R., Ripley, T. L., Borlikova, G., Stephens, D. N.,... Crews, F. T. (2004). Consequences of multiple withdrawals from alcohol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 28, 233-246. Eberl, C., Wiers, R. W., Pawelczack, S., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2013). Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 38-51. Eijnden, R. v. d., Van De Mheen, D., Vet, R., & Vermulst, A. (2011). Alcohol-specific parenting and adolescents' alcohol-related problems: The interacting role of alcohol availability at home and parental rules. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 72, 408. Eisinga, R., Grotenhuis, M. t., & Pelzer, B. (2013). The reliability of a two-item scale: Pearson, cronbach, or spearman-brown? International Journal of Public Health, 58, 637-642. Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19-23. Evenden, J. L. (1999). Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology, 146, 348-361. 180

Fergusson, D. M., Lynskey, M. T., & Horwood, L. (1994). Childhood exposure to alcohol and adolescent drinking patterns. Addiction, 89, 1007-1016. Fernie, G., Cole, J. C., Goudie, A. J., & Field, M. (2010). Risk-taking but not response inhibition or delay discounting predict alcohol consumption in social drinkers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 112, 54-61. Fernie, G., Peeters, M., Gullo, M. J., Christiansen, P., Cole, J. C., Sumnall, H., & Field, M. (2013). Multiple behavioural impulsivity tasks predict prospective alcohol involvement in adolescents. Addiction, 108, 1916-1923. Field, M., Caren, R., Fernie, G., & De Houwer, J. (2011). Alcohol approach tendencies in heavy drinkers: Comparison of effects in a relevant stimulus-response compatibility task and an approach/avoidance simon task. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 25, 697-701. Field, M., Christiansen, P., Cole, J., & Goudie, A. (2007). Delay discounting and the alcohol stroop in heavy drinking adolescents. Addiction, 102, 579-586. Field, M., & Eastwood, B. (2005). Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol. Psychopharmacology, 183, 350-357. Field, M., Kiernan, A., Eastwood, B., & Child, R. (2008). Rapid approach responses to alcohol cues in heavy drinkers. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39, 209-218. Field, M., Mogg, K., Zetteler, J., & Bradley, B. P. (2004). Attentional biases for alcohol cues in heavy and light social drinkers: The roles of initial orienting and maintained attention. Psychopharmacology, 176, 88-93. Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C., & O'reilly, R. C. (2004). By carrot or by stick: Cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science, 306, 1940-1943. doi:1102941 [pii] Franken, I. H., Kroon, L. Y., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. (2000). Selective cognitive processing of drug cues in heroin dependence. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 14, 395-400. 181

References Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions Cambridge University Press. Fromme, K., Stroot, E. A., & Kaplan, D. (1993). Comprehensive effects of alcohol: Development and psychometric assessment of a new expectancy questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 5, 19-26. Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., & Casey, B. (2006). Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 6885-6892. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692-731. Gladwin, T. E., Figner, B., Crone, E. A., & Wiers, R. W. (2011). Addiction, adolescence, and the integration of control and motivation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 364-376. Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581-586. Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1337-1345. Göritz, A. S., & Luthe, S. C. (2012). How do lotteries and study results influence response behavior in online panels? Social Science Computer Review, 31, 371-385. Graham, J. W. (2003). Adding missing-data-relevant variables to FIML-based structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 10, 80-100. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480. 182

Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. an improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197. Grenard, J. L., Ames, S. L., Wiers, R. W., Thush, C., Sussman, S., & Stacy, A. W. (2008). Working memory capacity moderates the predictive effects of drug-related associations on substance use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 22, 426-432. Hanson, K. L., Medina, K. L., Padula, C. B., Tapert, S. F., & Brown, S. A. (2011). Impact of adolescent alcohol and drug use on neuropsychological functioning in young adulthood: 10-year outcomes. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 20, 135-154. Harakeh, Z., & Vollebergh, W. A. (2012). The impact of active and passive peer influence on young adult smoking: An experimental study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 121, 220-223. Hermens, D. F., Lagopoulos, J., Tobias-Webb, J., De Regt, T., Dore, G., Juckes, L.,... Hickie, I. B. (2013). Pathways to alcohol-induced brain impairment in young people: A review. Cortex, 49, 3-17. Heyman, G. M. (2009). Addiction: A disorder of choice Harvard University Press. Hibell, B., Andersson, B., Bjarnason, T., Ahlström, S., Balakireva, O., Kokkevi, A.,... Morgan, M. (2004). The ESPAD report 2003 The Swedish Council for Information on Alcohol and Other Drugs (CAN), Council of Europe, Co-operation Group to Combat Drug Abuse and Illicit Trafficking in Drugs (Pompidou Group). Houben, K., Nosek, B. A., & Wiers, R. W. (2010). Seeing the forest through the trees: A comparison of different IAT variants measuring implicit alcohol associations. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 106, 204-211. Houben, K., & Wiers, R. W. (2008). Measuring implicit alcohol associations via the internet: Validation of web-based implicit association tests. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 1134-1143. 183

References Houben, K., & Wiers, R. W. (2009). Response inhibition moderates the relationship between implicit associations and drinking behavior. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33, 626-633. IBM Corporation. (2011). IBM SPSS statistics for windows<br /> (20.0th ed.). Armonk, NY: IBM Corporation. Jackson, K. M., O'Neill, S. E., & Sher, K. J. (2006). Characterizing alcohol dependence: Transitions during young and middle adulthood. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 14, 228-244. Janssen, T., Larsen, H., Peeters, M., Pronk, T., Vollebergh, W. A., & Wiers, R. W. (2014). Interactions between parental alcohol-specific rules and risk personalities in the prediction of adolescent alcohol use. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 49, 579-585. Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16-32. Kepper, A., Monshouwer, K., Van Dorsselaer, S., & Vollebergh, W. (2011). Substance use by adolescents in special education and residential youth care institutions. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 20, 311-319. Keren, G., & Schul, Y. (2009). Two is not always better than one A critical evaluation of twosystem theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 533-550. Khurana, A., Romer, D., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Giannetta, J. M., & Hurt, H. (2013). Working memory ability predicts trajectories of early alcohol use in adolescents: The mediational role of impulsivity. Addiction, 108, 506-515. King, K. M., & Chassin, L. (2007). A prospective study of the effects of age of initiation of alcohol and drug use on young adult substance dependence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 68, 256-265. 184

Kollins, S. H. (2003). Delay discounting is associated with substance use in college students. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 1167-1173. Koning, I. M., Harakeh, Z., Engels, R. C., & Vollebergh, W. A. (2010). A comparison of selfreported alcohol use measures by early adolescents: Questionnaires versus diary. Journal of Substance Use, 15, 166-173. Koning, I. M., Van den Eijnden, Regina J, Verdurmen, J. E., Engels, R. C., & Vollebergh, W. A. (2011). Long-term effects of a parent and student intervention on alcohol use in adolescents: A cluster randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 40, 541-547. Koning, I. M., Vollebergh, W. A., Smit, F., Verdurmen, J. E., Van Den Eijnden, Regina JJM, Ter Bogt, T. F.,... Engels, R. C. (2009). Preventing heavy alcohol use in adolescents (PAS): Cluster randomized trial of a parent and student intervention offered separately and simultaneously. Addiction, 104, 1669-1678. Köpetz, C. E., Lejuez, C. W., Wiers, R. W., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2013). Motivation and selfregulation in addiction A call for convergence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 3-24. Krank, M., Stewart, S. H., O'Connor, R., Woicik, P. B., Wall, A., & Conrod, P. J. (2011). Structural, concurrent, and predictive validity of the substance use risk profile scale in early adolescence. Addictive Behaviors, 36, 37-46. Krieglmeyer, R., & Deutsch, R. (2010). Comparing measures of approach avoidance behaviour: The manikin task vs. two versions of the joystick task. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 810-828. Krishnan-Sarin, S., Cavallo, D. A., Cooney, J. L., Schepis, T. S., Kong, G., Liss, T. B.,... Babuscio, T. (2013). An exploratory randomized controlled trial of a novel high-schoolbased smoking cessation intervention for adolescent smokers using abstinence contingent incentives and cognitive behavioral therapy. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 132, 346-351. 185

References Kypri, K., Gallagher, S. J., & Cashell-Smith, M. L. (2004). An internet-based survey method for college student drinking research. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 76, 45-53. Larsen, H., Overbeek, G., Vermulst, A. A., Granic, I., & Engels, R. C. (2010). Initiation and continuation of best friends and adolescents alcohol consumption: Do self-esteem and self-control function as moderators? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, 406-416. Larsen, H., Engels, R. C., Granic, I., & Overbeek, G. (2009). An experimental study on imitation of alcohol consumption in same-sex dyads. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 44, 250-255. doi:10.1093/alcalc/agp002 [doi] LeBourgeois, M. K., Giannotti, F., Cortesi, F., Wolfson, A. R., & Harsh, J. (2005). The relationship between reported sleep quality and sleep hygiene in italian and american adolescents. Pediatrics, 115, 257-265. Lejuez, C., Aklin, W. M., Jones, H. A., Richards, J. B., Strong, D. R., Kahler, C. W., & Read, J. P. (2003). The balloon analogue risk task (BART) differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11, 26-33. Lejuez, C., Aklin, W. M., Zvolensky, M. J., & Pedulla, C. M. (2003). Evaluation of the balloon analogue risk task (BART) as a predictor of adolescent real-world risk-taking behaviours. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 475-479. Lejuez, C., Aklin, W., Daughters, S., Zvolensky, M., Kahler, C., & Gwadz, M. (2007). Reliability and validity of the youth version of the balloon analogue risk task (BART Y) in the assessment of risk-taking behavior among inner-city adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 106-111. Lejuez, C., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L.,... Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The balloon analogue risk task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 75-84. 186

Lewis, J. M., Trinh, P., & Kirsh, D. (2011). A corpus analysis of strategy video game play in starcraft: Brood war. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 687-692. Loewenthal, K. M. (2001). An introduction to psychological tests and scales Psychology Press. Luna, B., Garver, K. E., Urban, T. A., Lazar, N. A., & Sweeney, J. A. (2004). Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Development, 75, 1357-1372. MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15-20. MacPherson, L., Magidson, J. F., Reynolds, E. K., Kahler, C. W., & Lejuez, C. (2010). Changes in sensation seeking and Risk Taking propensity predict increases in alcohol use among early adolescents. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 34, 1400-1408. Malmberg, M., Kleinjan, M., Overbeek, G., Vermulst, A. A., Lammers, J., & Engels, R. C. (2013). Are there reciprocal relationships between substance use risk personality profiles and alcohol or tobacco use in early adolescence? Addictive Behaviors, 38, 2851-2859. Malmberg, M., Kleinjan, M., Vermulst, A. A., Overbeek, G., Monshouwer, K., Lammers, J., & Engels, R. C. (2012). Do substance use risk personality dimensions predict the onset of substance use in early adolescence? A variable-and person-centered approach. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41, 1512-1525. Malmberg, M., Overbeek, G., Monshouwer, K., Lammers, J., Vollebergh, W. A., & Engels, R. C. (2010). Substance use risk profiles and associations with early substance use in adolescence. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 33, 474-485. Mares, S. H., Lichtwarck Aschoff, A., Burk, W. J., van der Vorst, H., & Engels, R. C. (2012). Parental alcohol specific rules and alcohol use from early adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 798-805. 187

References Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3-19. Miller, E. T., Neal, D. J., Roberts, L. J., Baer, J. S., Cressler, S. O., Metrik, J., & Marlatt, G. A. (2002). Test-retest reliability of alcohol measures: Is there a difference between internetbased assessment and traditional methods? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16, 56-63. Miller, J. W., Naimi, T. S., Brewer, R. D., & Jones, S. E. (2007). Binge drinking and associated health risk behaviors among high school students. Pediatrics, 119, 76-85. doi:119/1/76 [pii] Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100. Moffitt, T. E. (1993). The neuropsychology of conduct disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 135-151. Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Field, M., & De Houwer, J. (2003). Eye movements to smokingrelated pictures in smokers: Relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence. Addiction, 98, 825-836. Moskowitz, D. S., & Young, S. N. (2006). Ecological momentary assessment: What it is and why it is a method of the future in clinical psychopharmacology. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN, 31, 13-20. Muthén, L. (2014). Mplus discussion > > variances of growth factors. Retrieved from http://www.statmodel.com/discussion/messages/14/228.html?1395703447 Myerson, J., Green, L., & Warusawitharana, M. (2001). Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76, 235-243. Nasrallah, N. A., Yang, T. W., & Bernstein, I. L. (2009). Long-term risk preference and suboptimal decision making following adolescent alcohol use. Proceedings of the National 188

Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 17600-17604. doi:10.1073/pnas.0906629106; 10.1073/pnas.0906629106 Nees, F., Tzschoppe, J., Patrick, C. J., Vollstädt-Klein, S., Steiner, S., Poustka, L.,... Conrod, P. J. (2011). Determinants of early alcohol use in healthy adolescents: The differential contribution of neuroimaging and psychological factors. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37, 986-995. Newman, D. A. (2003). Longitudinal modeling with randomly and systematically missing data: A simulation of ad hoc, maximum likelihood, and multiple imputation techniques. Organizational Research Methods, 6, 328-362. Newman, J. P., & Kosson, D. S. (1986). Passive avoidance learning in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 252-256. Nigg, J. T., Wong, M. M., Martel, M. M., Jester, J. M., Puttler, L. I., Glass, J. M.,... Zucker, R. A. (2006). Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 468-475. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231. Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N. M., Devos, T., Ayala, A.,... Greenwald, A. G. (2009). National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 10593-10597. doi:10.1073/pnas.0809921106 [doi] Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A monte carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 535-569. 189

References O'Malley, P. M., & Wagenaar, A. C. (1991). Effects of minimum drinking age laws on alcohol use, related behaviors and traffic crash involvement among american youth: 1976-1987. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 52, 478-491. Oswald, D. P., Best, A. M., Coutinho, M. J., & Nagle, H. A. (2003). Trends in the special education identification rates of boys and girls: A call for research and change. Exceptionality, 11, 223-237. Pedersen, W., & Skrondal, A. (1998). Alcohol consumption debut: Predictors and consequences. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 59, 32-42. Peeters, M., Monshouwer, K., Janssen, T., Wiers, R. W., & Vollebergh, W. A. (2014). Working memory and alcohol use in at-risk adolescents: A 2-year Follow Up. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 38, 1176-1183. Peeters, M., Monshouwer, K., Schoot, R. A., Janssen, T., Vollebergh, W. A., & Wiers, R. W. (2013). Automatic processes and the drinking behavior in early adolescence: A prospective study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 37, 1737-1744. Peeters, M., Monshouwer, K., Schoot, R. v. d., Janssen, T., Vollebergh, W. A., & Wiers, R. W. (2014). Personality and the prediction of high-risk trajectories of alcohol use during adolescence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 75, 790. Peeters, M., Wiers, R. W., Monshouwer, K., Schoot, R., Janssen, T., & Vollebergh, W. A. (2012). Automatic processes in at risk adolescents: The role of alcohol approach tendencies and response inhibition in drinking behavior. Addiction, 107, 1939-1946. Peeters, M., Vollebergh, W. A., Wiers, R. W., & Field, M. (2014). Psychological changes and cognitive impairments in adolescent heavy drinkers. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 49, 182-186. doi:10.1093/alcalc/agt162 [doi] Petrides, M., & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal-and temporallobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20, 249-262. 190

Pieters, S., Burk, W. J., Van der Vorst, H., Engels, R. C., & Wiers, R. W. (2014). Impulsive and reflective processes related to alcohol use in young adolescents. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5 Pieters, S., Burk, W. J., Van der Vorst, H., Wiers, R. W., & Engels, R. C. (2012). The moderating role of working memory capacity and Alcohol Specific Rule Setting on the relation between approach tendencies and alcohol use in young adolescents. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 36, 915-922. Pieters, S., Van Der Zwaluw, C. S., Van Der Vorst, H., Wiers, R. W., Smeets, H., Lambrichs, E.,... Engels, R. C. (2012). The moderating effect of alcohol-specific parental rulesetting on the relation between the dopamine D2 receptor gene (DRD2), the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) and alcohol use in young adolescents. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 47, 663-670. doi:10.1093/alcalc/ags075 [doi] Pihl, R. O., Peterson, J., & Finn, P. R. (1990). Inherited predisposition to alcoholism: Characteristics of sons of male alcoholics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 291-301. Pleskac, T. J., Wallsten, T. S., Wang, P., & Lejuez, C. (2008). Development of an automatic response mode to improve the clinical utility of sequential risk-taking tasks. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16, 555-564. Reynolds, B., Karraker, K., Horn, K., & Richards, J. B. (2003). Delay and probability discounting as related to different stages of adolescent smoking and non-smoking. Behavioural Processes, 64, 333-344. Richards, J. B., Zhang, L., Mitchell, S. H., & Wit, H. (1999). Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: Effect of alcohol. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 121-143. Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2007). Approach and avoidance in fear of spiders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 105-120. Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2001). Incentive sensitization and addiction. Addiction, 96, 103-114. 191

References Ross, T. P., Hanouskova, E., Giarla, K., Calhoun, E., & Tucker, M. (2007). The reliability and validity of the self-ordered pointing task. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 449-458. Rubin, D. B. (1996). Multiple imputation after 18 years. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 91, 473-489. Rutter, M., Dunn, J., Plomin, R., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Maughan, B.,... Eaves, L. (1997). Integrating nature and nurture: Implications of person-environment correlations and interactions for developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 335-364. Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science (New York, N.Y.), 275(5306), 1593-1599. Sher, K. J., Walitzer, K. S., Wood, P. K., & Brent, E. E. (1991). Characteristics of children of alcoholics: Putative risk factors, substance use and abuse, and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 427-448. Skeel, R. L., Neudecker, J., Pilarski, C., & Pytlak, K. (2007). The utility of personality variables and behaviorally-based measures in the prediction of risk-taking behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 203-214. Skeel, R. L., Pilarski, C., Pytlak, K., & Neudecker, J. (2008). Personality and performance-based measures in the prediction of alcohol use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 22, 402-409. Sobell, L. C., & Sobell, M. B. (1992). Timeline follow-back: A technique for assessing selfreported alcohol consumption. In R. Z. Litten, & J. P. Allen (Eds.), Measuring alcohol consumption: Psychosocial and biochemical methods (pp. 41-72). Totowa, NJ, US: Humana Press. Squeglia, L. M., Schweinsburg, A. D., Pulido, C., & Tapert, S. F. (2011). Adolescent binge drinking linked to abnormal spatial working memory brain activation: Differential gender effects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35, 1831-1841. 192

Squeglia, L. M., Spadoni, A. D., Infante, M. A., Myers, M. G., & Tapert, S. F. (2009). Initiating moderate to heavy alcohol use predicts changes in neuropsychological functioning for adolescent girls and boys. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23, 715-722. Sriram, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2009). The brief implicit association test. Experimental Psychology (Formerly Zeitschrift Für Experimentelle Psychologie), 56, 283-294. Stacy, A. W. (1997). Memory activation and expectancy as prospective predictors of alcohol and marijuana use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 61-73. Stacy, A. W., & Wiers, R. W. (2010). Implicit cognition and addiction: A tool for explaining paradoxical behavior. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 551-575. Steinberg, L. (2004). Risk taking in adolescence: What changes, and why? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 51-58. Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence new perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 55-59. Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78-106. Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764-1778. Steinberg, L., & Monahan, K. C. (2007). Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1531-1543. Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Adolescent development. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 2, 55-87. 193

References Stephens, D. N., & Duka, T. (2008). Cognitive and emotional consequences of binge drinking: Role of amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.Series B, Biological Sciences, 363, 3169-3179. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0097 [doi] Sterne, J. A., White, I. R., Carlin, J. B., Spratt, M., Royston, P., Kenward, M. G.,... Carpenter, J. R. (2009). Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: Potential and pitfalls. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.), 338, b2393. doi:10.1136/bmj.b2393 [doi] Stewart, S. H., Conrod, P. J., Marlatt, G. A., Comeau, M. N., Thush, C., & Krank, M. (2005). New developments in prevention and early intervention for alcohol abuse in youths. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29, 278-286. Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review : An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc, 8, 220-247. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1 Stroebe, W., van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Papies, E. K., & Aarts, H. (2013). Why most dieters fail but some succeed: A goal conflict model of eating behavior. Psychological Review, 120, 110-138. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662. Synnaeve, G., & Bessiere, P. (2011). A bayesian model for RTS units control applied to StarCraft. Computational Intelligence and Games (CIG), 2011 IEEE Conference On, 190-196. Tapert, S. F., Granholm, E., Leedy, N. G., & Brown, S. A. (2002). Substance use and withdrawal: Neuropsychological functioning over 8 years in youth. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 873-883. Thush, C., Wiers, R. W., Ames, S. L., Grenard, J. L., Sussman, S., & Stacy, A. W. (2008). Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 94, 116-124. 194

Toplak, M. E., Bucciarelli, S. M., Jain, U., & Tannock, R. (2008). Executive functions: Performance-based measures and the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF) in adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 15, 53-72. Townshend, J. M., & Duka, T. (2005). Binge drinking, cognitive performance and mood in a population of young social drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29, 317-325. Van Buuren, S., Boshuizen, H., & Knook, D. (2000). Multivariate imputation by chained equations. MICE V1.0 user's manual. Leiden: TNO Preventie en Gezondheid. Van Buuren, S., Boshuizen, H. C., & Knook, D. L. (1999). Multiple imputation of missing blood pressure covariates in survival analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 18, 681-694. Van Der Vorst, H., Engels, R. C., Deković, M., Meeus, W., & Vermulst, A. A. (2007). Alcoholspecific rules, personality and adolescents' alcohol use: A longitudinal person environment study. Addiction, 102, 1064-1075. Van Der Vorst, H., Engels, R. C., Meeus, W., & Deković, M. (2006). The impact of alcoholspecific rules, parental norms about early drinking and parental alcohol use on adolescents drinking behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1299-1306. Van Der Vorst, H., Engels, R. C., Meeus, W., Deković, M., & Van Leeuwe, J. (2005). The role of alcohol specific socialization in adolescents drinking behaviour. Addiction, 100, 1464-1476. Van Der Vorst, H., Vermulst, A. A., Meeus, W. H., Deković, M., & Engels, R. C. (2009). Identification and prediction of drinking trajectories in early and mid-adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 38, 329-341. van der Zwaluw, Carmen S, Larsen, H., & Engels, R. C. (2012). Best friends and alcohol use in adolescence: The role of the dopamine D4 receptor gene. Addiction Biology, 17, 1036-1045. 195

References Van Dorsselaer, S., De Looze, M., Vermeulen-Smit, E., De Roos, S., Verdurmen, J., Ter Bogt, T.,... Vollebergh, W. (2009). HBSC 2009 gezondheid, welzijn en opvoeding van jongeren in nederland. Utrecht: Trimbos-instituut. Van Dorsselaer, S., De Looze, M., Vermeulen-Smit, E., De Roos, S., Verdurmen, J., Ter Bogt, T.,... Vollebergh, W. (2013). HBSC 2013 gezondheid, welzijn en opvoeding van jongeren in nederland. Utrecht: Trimbos-instituut. van Ginkel, J. R., & Kroonenberg, P. M. (2014). Using generalized procrustes analysis for multiple imputation in principal component analysis. Journal of Classification, 31, 242-269. van Hemel-Ruiter, M. E., de Jong, P. J., Oldehinkel, A. J., & Ostafin, B. D. (2013). Rewardrelated attentional biases and adolescent substance use: The TRAILS study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27, 142-150. van Hemel-Ruiter, M. E., de Jong, P. J., Ostafin, B. D., & Wiers, R. W. (2014). Reward sensitivity, attentional bias, and executive control in early adolescent alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors, 40, 84-90. van Hemel-Ruiter, M. E., de Jong, P. J., & Wiers, R. W. (2011). Appetitive and regulatory processes in young adolescent drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 36, 18-26. Van Laar, M. W., Cruts, A. A. N., van Ooyen-Houben, M. M. J., Meijer, R. F., Brunt, T., Croes, E. A.,... Ketelaars, A. P. M. (2011). National drug monitor. Utrecht: Trimbos Institute. Van Leijenhorst, L., Moor, B. G., Op de Macks, Zdeňa A, Rombouts, S. A., Westenberg, P. M., & Crone, E. A. (2010). Adolescent risky decision-making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. NeuroImage, 51, 345-355. Van Zundert, R. M., Van Der Vorst, H., Vermulst, A. A., & Engels, R. (2006). Pathways to alcohol use among dutch students in regular education and education for adolescents with behavioral problems: The role of parental alcohol use, general parenting practices, and alcohol-specific parenting practices. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 456-467. 196

Verdejo-García, A., Lawrence, A. J., & Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 777-810. Warner, L. A., White, H. R., & Johnson, V. (2007). Alcohol initiation experiences and family history of alcoholism as predictors of problem-drinking trajectories. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 68, 56-65. Waters, A. J., Sayette, M. A., & Wertz, J. M. (2003). Carry-over effects can modulate emotional stroop effects. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 501-509. Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale Fourth edition (WAIS IV). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson, Wechsler, H., & Nelson, T. F. (2001). Binge drinking and the american college students: What's five drinks? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 287-291. White, H. R., Marmorstein, N. R., Crews, F. T., Bates, M. E., Mun, E., & Loeber, R. (2011). Associations between heavy drinking and changes in impulsive behavior among adolescent boys. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35, 295-303. White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Bartusch, D. J., Needles, D. J., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1994). Measuring impulsivity and examining its relationship to delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 192-205. Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669-689. Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2003). Understanding the role of impulsivity and externalizing psychopathology in alcohol abuse: Application of the UPPS impulsive behavior scale. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11, 210-217. 197

References Wiers, R. W., Bartholow, B. D., van den Wildenberg, E., Thush, C., Engels, R. C., Sher, K. J.,... Stacy, A. W. (2007). Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: A review and a model. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 86, 263-283. Wiers, R. W., Beckers, L., Houben, K., & Hofmann, W. (2009). A short fuse after alcohol: Implicit power associations predict aggressiveness after alcohol consumption in young heavy drinkers with limited executive control. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93, 300-305. Wiers, R. W., Boelema, S., Nicolau, K., & Gladwin, T. E. (in press). On the development of implicit and control processes in relation to substance use in adolescence. Current Addiction Reports. Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients' approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychological Science, 22, 490-497. doi:10.1177/0956797611400615 [doi] Wiers, R. W., Gladwin, T. E., Hofmann, W., Salemink, E., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2013). Cognitive bias modification and cognitive control training in addiction and related psychopathology mechanisms, clinical perspectives, and ways forward. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 192-212. Wiers, R. W., Hoogeveen, K., Sergeant, J. A., & Gunning, W. B. (1997). High and low dose alcohol related expectancies and the differential associations with drinking in male and female adolescents and young adults. Addiction, 92, 871-888. Wiers, R. W., Rinck, M., Dictus, M., & Van Den Wildenberg, E. (2009). Relatively strong automatic appetitive action tendencies in male carriers of the OPRM1 g allele. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 8, 101-106. Wiers, R. W., & Stacy, A. W. (2006). Implicit cognition and addiction. Current Directions in 198 Psychological Science, 15, 292-296.

Wiers, R. W., Van De Luitgaarden, J., Van Den Wildenberg, E., & Smulders, F. T. (2005). Challenging implicit and explicit alcohol related cognitions in young heavy drinkers. Addiction, 100, 806-819. Wiers, R. W., Van Woerden, N., Smulders, F. T., & De Jong, P. J. (2002). Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 648-658. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24. Woicik, P. A., Stewart, S. H., Pihl, R. O., & Conrod, P. J. (2009). The substance use risk profile scale: A scale measuring traits linked to reinforcement-specific substance use profiles. Addictive Behaviors, 34, 1042-1055. Wood, M. D., Read, J. P., Mitchell, R. E., & Brand, N. H. (2004). Do parents still matter? parent and peer influences on alcohol involvement among recent high school graduates. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 18, 19-30. Yu, J. (2003). The association between parental alcohol-related behaviors and children's drinking. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 69, 253-262. Zucker, R. A. (2008). Anticipating problem alcohol use developmentally from childhood into middle adulthood: What have we learned? Addiction, 103, 100-108. Zuckerman, M., Eysenck, S. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1978). Sensation seeking in england and america: Cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 139-149. Zvielli, A., Bernstein, A., & Koster, E. H. (2014). Temporal dynamics of attentional bias. Clinical Psychological Science, doi:10.1177/2167702614551572 199

SUMMARY IN DUTCH

De algemene doelen van deze dissertatie waren om te onderzoeken of specifiek geselecteerde cognitieve- en omgevingsfactoren samengingen met de ontwikkeling van middelengebruik gedurende adolescentie. Adolescentie wordt vaak beschreven als een cruciale en risicovolle ontwikkelingsperiode waarin jongeren een toegenomen neiging tot risicogedrag vertonen. Het is in eerder onderzoek aangetoond dat middelengebruik samenhangt met verschillen in hersenfunctie, executief functioneren (oftewel het vermogen om gedrag te beheersen en te reguleren) en andere onderdelen van cognitie. Het is echter niet altijd duidelijk of deze verschillen voorafgaan aan middelengebruik, of ontstaan als gevolg van middelengebruik. Er zijn via recent onderzoek een aantal kernprocessen onderscheiden die in het bijzonder lijken samen te hangen met middelengebruik. Zo hebben strikte ouderregels omtrent alcoholgebruik invloed op alcoholgebruik tijdens en na adolescentie. Daarnaast zijn er risicodragende externaliserende (oorzaak zoekende buiten jezelf) en internaliserende (oorzaak zoekende binnen jezelf) persoonlijkheidstrekken die allen samenhangen met het ontstaan van middelengebruik. Ook afwijkingen in executief functioneren hangen samen met grotere hoeveelheden alcoholgebruik tijdens adolescentie. Tenslotte zijn er cognitieve afwijkingen die worden geactiveerd door bepaalde signalen uit de omgeving (denk aan geluiden en geuren die samengaan met restaurant- of barbezoek) en samengaan met een automatische gedragsverandering ten faveure van middelengebruik, die zo de kans op middelengebruik tijdens de adolescentie verhogen. Een centrale vraag in deze dissertatie is of de ontwikkeling van deze kernprocessen samengaat met de ontwikkeling van andere kernprocessen en met toename in middelengebruik. De empirische (onderzoeksresultaat-bevattende) hoofdstukken in deze dissertatie dekken deze kernprocessen af met vijf specifieke onderzoeksvragen. We gebruikten data uit een internetgebaseerd langetermijnonderzoek met vier meetpunten gespreid over twee jaar, om op deze vragen antwoord te zoeken. Bij elk meetpunt werden verschillende cognitieve processen, persoonlijkheidstrekken en omgevingsfactoren, alsook de hoeveelheid middelengebruik (met name alcohol-, tabaks- en marijuanagebruik) gemeten. Allereerst onderzochten we de rol van een belangrijke omgevingsfactor, namelijk alcoholspecifieke ouderregels, en de rol een belangrijke persoonsgerichte factor, namelijk persoonlijkheidstrekken, en de rol van een combinatie van de twee (denk: een persoonlijkheidstrek die zich vooral uit als er weinig ouderregels zijn), in de voorspelling van het ontstaan en verergeren van alcoholgebruik. Op basis van eerder onderzoek verwachtten wij dat er minder bewijs voor alcoholgebruik zou zijn als er strengere ouderregels zouden zijn. Daarnaast verwachtten we dat er meer bewijs voor alcoholgebruik zou zijn als deelnemers 203

Summary in Dutch hoger scoorden op risico-geassocieerde persoonlijkheidstrekken. Op basis van de onderzoeksresultaten concludeerden wij dat hogere scores op persoonlijkheidstrekken sensatiezucht en hopeloosheid, en laag-strikte (lakse) ouderregels voorspellend waren voor meer alcoholgebruik bij latere metingen. Er was echter geen bewijs dat bij deze deelnemers de invloed van risico-geassocieerde persoonlijkheidstrekken afhing van hoe streng de ouderregels waren. Ten tweede onderzochten we de rol van zelf aangegeven (in een vragenlijst die door de deelnemer zelf wordt ingevuld) als indirect gemeten (zoals met een computertaak die het gedrag van de deelnemer beoordeelt) onderdelen van impulsiviteit en sensatiezucht en hun samenhang met het ontstaan van middelengebruik. Op basis van eerder onderzoek verwachtten wij dat zowel zelf aangegeven als indirect gemeten onderdelen ieder op hun eigen manier voorspellen zouden zijn voor het ontstaan van middelengebruik. Wij bevonden dat, als de invloed van leeftijd en geslacht werd uitgesloten, voornamelijk zelf aangegeven onderdelen van sensatiezucht voorspellend waren voor het ontstaan van middelengebruik gedurende de meetperiode. Indirect gemeten onderdelen leken geen grote rol te hebben. Ten derde onderzochten we de rol van twee indirect gemeten onderdelen van executief functioneren (werkgeheugencapaciteit en responsinhibitie, oftewel het vermogen een initiële reactie niet door te zetten) en hun verband met het ontstaan van alcoholgebruik en binge-drinken. Op basis van eerder onderzoek verwachtten wij dat slechte prestaties op beide onderdelen voorspellend zouden zijn voor het ontstaan van alcoholgebruik en binge-drinken op jongere leeftijd. De resultaten van ons onderzoek lieten zien dat slechte prestaties op deze twee onderdelen inderdaad voorspellend waren voor een vroeger ontstaan van alcoholgebruik, terwijl specifiek werkgeheugencapaciteit voorspellend was voor een vroeger ontstaan van binge-drinken. Ten vierde onderzochten wij langetermijnrelaties tussen onderdelen van impulsiviteit en indirect gemeten alcoholspecifieke cognitieve afwijkingen, en de langetermijnrelatie tussen deze cognitieve afwijkingen en later optredende verschillen in alcoholgebruik. Op basis van eerder onderzoek verwachtten wij dat bij meer blijk van impulsiviteit en meer alcoholgebruik, de kans op aanwezigheid van cognitieve afwijkingen zou toenemen. Daarnaast verwachtten we dat een grotere hoeveelheid cognitieve afwijking, op lange termijn voorspellend zou zijn voor een toename in alcoholgebruik. We bevonden dat hogere scores op een maat voor cognitieve afwijkingen in de aandacht (de zogeheten visuele-probe-taak) op lange termijn voorspellend waren voor een grotere mate van alcoholgebruik bij onze deelnemers. Echter waren er geen 204

indicaties dat eerder alcoholgebruik of eerdere hoge scores op onderdelen van impulsiviteit, op lange termijn voorspellend waren voor een toename in cognitieve afwijkingen. Ten vijfde onderzochten we of de ontwikkeling van indirect gemeten toenaderingsafwijkingen (oftewel de automatische neiging om alcoholbronnen te benaderen en te consumeren) samenhing met de ontwikkeling van alcoholgebruik. Op basis van eerder onderzoek voorspelden wij dat het intercept van cognitieve afwijkingen (oftewel verschillen in het basisniveau aan cognitieve afwijkingen) zou samenhangen met zowel het intercept van alcoholgebruik, als de toename van alcoholgebruik gedurende het verloop van het onderzoek. Ook voorspelden wij dat de toename in cognitieve afwijkingen zou samenhangen met het intercept van alcoholgebruik en met de toename in alcoholgebruik. Ten slotte voorspelden wij dat deze samenhangen sterker zouden zijn (zodat verschillen in cognitieve afwijkingen en verschillen in alcoholgebruik meer met elkaar in overeenstemming zouden zijn) als die deelnemer slechte prestaties op gebied van werkgeheugencapaciteit liet zien. Wij bevonden dat het intercept van cognitieve afwijkingen samenhing met het intercept van alcoholgebruik, maar dat er geen indicaties waren voor samenhang tussen toenames in elk proces. Bovendien waren er geen indicaties dat de aard van deze samenhang afhing van werkgeheugencapaciteit. In het algemeen concludeerden wij dat er bewijs was dat individuele verschillen in zowel indirect gemeten, als zelf aangegeven cognitieve processen voorspellend waren voor verschillen in middelengebruik. Echter was er weinig bewijs bij deze deelnemers dat het omgekeerde ook gebeurde: dat eerder alcoholgebruik voorspellend was voor veranderingen in deze cognitieve processen. Interessant genoeg was dit ook het geval bij cognitieve afwijkingen, waarvan over het algemeen wordt aangenomen dat ze het gevolg zijn van beloonde leerervaringen na het innemen van middelen. In deze dissertatie geven wij echter ook aan dat problemen met het accuraat meten van indirect gemeten processen mogelijk hier invloed gehad op kunnen hebben. 205

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

In Memoriam This dissertation is dedicated to the memory of Mark D. Wood, recently and suddenly taken from life at age 54. As father, mentor, professor, life enthusiast, generous and hospitable friend, loving husband and diligent academic, he improved and enriched the lives of family, friends, and colleagues. I will miss him always.